Funny Vine Look at This Graph
The Dunning–Kruger effect is a cognitive bias[2] whereby people with low ability, expertise, or experience regarding a certain type of a task or area of knowledge tend to overestimate their ability or knowledge. Some researchers also include in their definition the opposite effect for high performers: their tendency to underestimate their skills. The Dunning–Kruger effect is usually measured by comparing self-assessment with objective performance. For example, the participants in a study may be asked to complete a quiz and then estimate how well they did. This subjective assessment is then compared with how well they actually did. This can happen either in relative or in absolute terms, i.e., in comparison with one's peer group as the percentage of peers outperformed or in comparison with objective standards as the number of questions answered correctly. The Dunning–Kruger effect appears in both cases but is more pronounced in relative terms: the bottom quartile of performers tend to see themselves as being part of the top two quartiles. The initial study was published by David Dunning and Justin Kruger in 1999. It focuses on logical reasoning, grammar, and social skills. Since then, various other studies have been conducted across a wide range of tasks. These include skills from fields such as business, politics, medicine, driving, aviation, spatial memory, exams in school, and literacy.
The Dunning–Kruger effect is usually explained in terms of meta-cognitive abilities. This approach is based on the idea that poor performers have not yet acquired the ability to distinguish between good and bad performances. They tend to overrate themselves because they do not see the qualitative difference between their performances and the performances of others. This has also been termed the "dual-burden account" since the lack of skill is paired with the ignorance of this lack. Some researchers include the meta-cognitive component as part of the definition of the Dunning–Kruger effect and not just as an explanation distinct from it.
Many debates surrounding the Dunning–Kruger effect and criticisms of it focus on the meta-cognitive explanation without denying the empirical findings. This is often done by providing alternative explanations that promise a better account of the observed tendencies. The most prominent among them is the statistical explanation, which holds that the Dunning–Kruger effect is mainly a statistical artifact due to the regression toward the mean combined with another cognitive bias known as the better-than-average effect. Other theorists hold that the way low and high performers are distributed makes it more difficult for low performers to assess their skill level, thereby explaining their erroneous self-assessments independent of their meta-cognitive abilities. Another account sees the lack of incentives to give accurate self-assessments as the source of error.
The Dunning–Kruger effect has been described as relevant for various practical matters but there are disagreements about the magnitude of its influence. Inaccurate self-assessment can lead people to make bad decisions, such as choosing a career for which they are unfit or engaging in behavior dangerous for themselves or others due to being unaware of lacking the necessary skills. It may also inhibit the affected from addressing their shortcomings to improve themselves. In some cases, the associated overconfidence may have positive side effects, like increasing motivation and energy.
Definition [edit]
The Dunning-Kruger effect is defined as the tendency of people with low ability in a specific area to give overly positive assessments of this ability.[3] [4] [5] This is often understood as a cognitive bias, i.e. as a systematic tendency to engage in erroneous forms of thinking and judging.[2] [6] [7] Biases are systematic in the sense that they occur consistently in different situations.[6] They are tendencies since they concern certain inclinations or dispositions that may be observed in groups of people but are not manifested in every performance.[2] [6] In the case of the Dunning-Kruger effect, this applies mainly to people with low skill in a specific area trying to evaluate their competence within this area. The systematic error concerns their tendency to greatly overestimate their competence or to see themselves as more skilled than they are.[2]
Some researchers emphasize the meta-cognitive component in their definition. On this view, the Dunning-Kruger effect is the thesis that those who are incompetent in a given area tend to be ignorant of their incompetence, i.e. they lack the meta-cognitive ability to become aware of their incompetence.[1] [2] This definition lends itself to a simple explanation of the effect: incompetence often includes being unable to tell the difference between competence and incompetence, which is why it is difficult for the incompetent to recognize their incompetence.[1] [2] This is sometimes termed the "dual-burden" account since two burdens come paired: the lack of skill and the ignorance of this lack.[8] But most definitions focus on the tendency to overestimate one's ability and see the relation to meta-cognition as a possible explanation independent of one's definition.[8] [9] [2] This distinction is relevant since the meta-cognitive explanation is controversial and various criticisms of the Dunning-Kruger effect target this explanation but not the effect itself when defined in the narrow sense.[8] [3] [9]
The Dunning-Kruger effect is usually defined specifically for the self-assessments of people with a low level of competence.[2] [1] [8] But some definitions do not restrict it to the bias of people with low skill and instead see it as pertaining to false self-evaluations on different skill levels.[10] So it is sometimes claimed that it includes the reverse effect for people with high skill.[3] [8] [5] On this view, the Dunning-Kruger effect also concerns the tendency of highly skilled people to underestimate their abilities relative to the abilities of others. But it has been argued that the source of this error is not the self-assessment of one's skills but an overly positive assessment of the skills of others.[3] This phenomenon has been categorized as a form of the false-consensus effect.[3] [8]
Measurement and analysis [edit]
The most common approach to measuring the Dunning-Kruger effect is to compare self-assessment with objective performance. The self-assessment is sometimes called subjective ability in contrast to the objective ability corresponding to the actual performance.[7] The self-assessment may be done before or after the performance.[7] [3] [8] If done afterward, it is important that the participants receive no independent clues during the performance as to how well they did. So if the activity involves answering quiz questions, no feedback is given as to whether a given answer was correct.[3] The measurement of the subjective and the objective ability can be in absolute or relative terms. When done in absolute terms, self-assessment and performance are measured according to absolute standards, e.g. concerning how many quiz questions were answered correctly.[1] [9] When done in relative terms, the results are compared with a peer group. In this case, each participant is asked to assess their performance in relation to the other participants, for example in the form of estimating the percentage of peers they outperformed.[3] [1] The Dunning-Kruger effect is present in both cases but tends to be significantly more pronounced when done in relative terms. So people are usually more accurate when predicting their raw score than when assessing how well they did relative to their peer group.[1]
The main point of interest for researchers is usually the correlation between subjective and objective ability.[7] In order to provide a simplified form of analysis of the measurements, objective performances are often divided into four groups, starting from the bottom quartile of low performers to the top quartile of high performers.[1] [3] [7] The strongest effect is seen for the participants in the bottom quartile, who tend to see themselves as being part of the top two quartiles when measured in relative terms.[1] Some researchers focus their analysis on the difference between the two abilities, i.e. on subjective ability minus objective ability, to highlight the negative correlation.[7]
Studies [edit]
The Dunning-Kruger effect has been researched in many different studies across a wide range of tasks.[1] [2] The initial study focused on logical reasoning, grammar skills, and social abilities, like emotional intelligence and judging which jokes are funny.[1] [2] While many studies are conducted in labs, others take place in real-world settings. The latter include assessing the knowledge hunters have of firearms and safety or laboratory technicians' knowledge of medical lab procedures.[1] More recent studies have also engaged in large-scale attempts to collect the relevant data online.[9] Various studies focus on students—for example, to self-assess their performance just after completing an exam. In some cases, these studies gather and compare data from many different countries.[1] Other fields of research include business, politics, medicine, driving skills, aviation, spatial memory, literacy, debating skills, and chess.[2] [1] [5] [10] [8]
The psychological phenomenon of illusory superiority was identified as a form of cognitive bias in Kruger and Dunning's 1999 study "Unskilled and Unaware of It: How Difficulties in Recognizing One's Own Incompetence Lead to Inflated Self-Assessments".[11]
Other investigations of the phenomenon, such as "Why People Fail to Recognize Their Own Incompetence",[12] indicate that much incorrect self-assessment of competence derives from the person's ignorance of a given activity's standards of performance. Dunning and Kruger's research also indicates that training in a task, such as solving a logic puzzle, increases people's ability to accurately evaluate how good they are at it.[13]
In Self-insight: Roadblocks and Detours on the Path to Knowing Thyself,[14] Dunning described the Dunning–Kruger effect as "the anosognosia of everyday life", referring to a neurological condition in which a disabled person either denies or seems unaware of their disability. He stated: "If you're incompetent, you can't know you're incompetent ... The skills you need to produce a right answer are exactly the skills you need to recognize what a right answer is."[15]
In 2011, Dunning wrote about his observations that people with substantial, measurable deficits in their knowledge or expertise lack the ability to recognize those deficits and, therefore, despite potentially making error after error, tend to think they are performing competently when they are not: "In short, those who are incompetent, for lack of a better term, should have little insight into their incompetence—an assertion that has come to be known as the Dunning–Kruger effect".[16] In 2014, Dunning and Helzer described how the Dunning–Kruger effect "suggests that poor performers are not in a position to recognize the shortcomings in their performance".[17]
Dunning and Kruger tested the hypotheses of the cognitive bias of illusory superiority on undergraduate students of introductory courses in psychology by examining the students' self-assessments of their intellectual skills in inductive, deductive, and abductive logical reasoning, English grammar, and personal sense of humor. After learning their self-assessment scores, the students were asked to estimate their ranks in the psychology class. The competent students underestimated their class rank, and the incompetent students overestimated theirs, but the incompetent students did not estimate their class rank as higher than the ranks estimated by the competent group. Across four studies, the research indicated that the study participants who scored in the bottom quartile on tests of their sense of humor, knowledge of grammar, and logical reasoning overestimated their test performance and their abilities; despite test scores that placed them in the 12th percentile, the participants estimated they ranked in the 62nd percentile.[11]
Moreover, competent students tended to underestimate their own competence, because they erroneously presumed that tasks easy for them to perform were also easy for other people to perform. Incompetent students improved their ability to estimate their class rank correctly after receiving minimal tutoring in the skills they previously lacked, regardless of any objective improvement gained in said skills of perception.[11] The 2004 study "Mind-Reading and Metacognition: Narcissism, not Actual Competence, Predicts Self-estimated Ability"[18] extended the cognitive-bias premise of illusory superiority to test subjects' emotional sensitivity toward other people and their own perceptions of other people.
The 2003 study "How Chronic Self-Views Influence (and Potentially Mislead) Estimates of Performance"[19] indicated a shift in the participants' view of themselves when influenced by external cues. The participants' knowledge of geography was tested; some tests were intended to affect the participants' self-view positively, and some were intended to affect it negatively. The participants then were asked to rate their performances; the participants given tests with a positive intent reported better performance than did the participants given tests with a negative intent.
To test Dunning and Kruger's hypotheses "that people, at all performance levels, are equally poor at estimating their relative performance", the 2006 study "Skilled or Unskilled, but Still Unaware of It: How Perceptions of Difficulty Drive Miscalibration in Relative Comparisons"[20] investigated three studies that manipulated the "perceived difficulty of the tasks, and, hence, [the] participants' beliefs about their relative standing". The investigation indicated that when the experimental subjects were presented with moderately difficult tasks, there was little variation among the best performers and the worst performers in their ability to predict their performance accurately. With more difficult tasks, the best performers were less accurate in predicting their performance than were the worst performers. Therefore, judges at all levels of skill are subject to similar degrees of error in the performance of tasks.
In testing alternative explanations for the cognitive bias of illusory superiority, the 2008 study "Why the Unskilled are Unaware: Further Explorations of (Absent) Self-insight Among the Incompetent"[21] reached the same conclusions as previous studies of the Dunning–Kruger effect: that, in contrast to high performers, "poor performers do not learn from feedback suggesting a need to improve".
Explanations [edit]
Meta-cognitive [edit]
Various explanations have been proposed to account for the Dunning-Kruger effect. The initial and most common account is based on meta-cognitive abilities.[2] [1] [9] It rests on the assumption that part of acquiring a skill consists in learning to distinguish between good and bad performances of this skill. Since people with low skill have not yet acquired this discriminatory ability, they are unable to properly assess their performance.[1] [2] [7] This leads them to believe that they are better than they are because they do not see the qualitative difference between their performances and performances by others. So they lack the meta-cognitive ability to recognize their incompetence.[1] [2] This account has also been called the "dual-burden account" or the "double-burden of incompetence", since the burden of regular incompetence is paired with the burden of meta-cognitive incompetence.[8] [1] [9] It is usually combined with the thesis that the relevant meta-cognitive abilities are acquired as one's skill level increases.[10] But the meta-cognitive lack may also hinder some people from becoming better by hiding their flaws from them.[1] This can then be used to explain how self-confidence is sometimes higher for unskilled people than for people with an average skill: only the latter are aware of their flaws.[10] [1] Some attempts have been made to measure meta-cognitive abilities directly to confirm this hypothesis. The findings suggest that there is a reduced meta-cognitive sensitivity among poor performers but it is not clear that its extent is sufficient to explain the Dunning-Kruger effect.[8] An indirect argument for the meta-cognitive account is based on the observation that training people in logical reasoning helps them make more accurate self-assessments.[3]
Criticism and alternatives [edit]
Not everyone agrees with the assumptions on which the meta-cognitive account is based.[9] Many criticisms of the Dunning-Kruger effect have the meta-cognitive account as their main focus but agree otherwise with the empirical findings themselves.[1] This line of argument usually proceeds by providing an alternative approach that promises a better explanation of the observed tendencies. Some explanations focus only on one specific factor while others see a combination of various factors as the source.[1] One such account is based on the idea that both low and high performers have in general the same meta-cognitive ability to assess their skill level.[22] But given the assumption that the skill levels of many low performers are very close to each other, i.e., that "many people [are] piled up at the bottom rungs of skill level",[3] they find themselves in a more difficult position to assess their skills in relation to their peers.[22] [8] So the reason for the increased tendency to give false self-assessments is not a lack in meta-cognitive ability but a more challenging situation in which this ability is applied.[22] Thus the increased error can be explained without a dual-burden account.[3] [8] One criticism of this approach is directed against the assumption that this type of distribution of skill levels can always be used as an explanation. While it can be found in various fields where the Dunning-Kruger effect has been researched, it is not present in all of them.[3] Another criticism rests on the fact that this account can explain the Dunning-Kruger effect only when the self-assessment is measured relative to one's peer group, not when measured relative to absolute standards.[3]
Another account, sometimes given by theorists with an economic background, focuses on the fact that participants in the corresponding studies usually lack the incentive to give accurate self-assessments.[1] [23] In such cases, the participants may be motivated by intellectual laziness or a desire to look good in the eyes of the experimenter to give overly positive self-assessments. For this reason, some studies were conducted with additional incentives to be accurate. In one study, for example, a monetary reward was given to a group of participants based on how accurate their self-assessment was. But these studies failed to show any significant increase in accuracy for the incentive group in contrast to the control group.[1]
A different approach is further removed from psychological explanations and sees the Dunning-Kruger effect as mainly a statistical artifact without reference to any prominent underlying psychological tendencies.[7] [1] [24] It is based on the idea that the statistical effect known as regression toward the mean is sufficient to account for the empirical findings. In the case of the quality of performances, this effect rests on the idea that the quality of a given performance depends not just on the agent's skill level but also on the good or bad luck involved on an occasion.[7] [1] So even if a participant with average skill gives an accurate self-assessment of their skill, their performance may be unlucky on this occasion, causing them to fall into the category of low performers who overestimated their skill. According to this approach, the randomness of luck is blamed for the discrepancy between self-assessed ability and objective performance, especially in extreme cases.[7] [1]
Most researchers acknowledge that regression toward the mean is a relevant statistical effect that has to be taken into account when interpreting the empirical findings. This can be achieved by various methods.[8] [1] But such adjustments do not eliminate the Dunning-Kruger effect, which is why the view that regression toward the mean is sufficient to explain it is usually rejected.[9] However, it has been suggested that, when paired with other cognitive biases, like the better-than-average effect, one can provide an almost complete explanation of the empirical findings.[7] [8] [3] This type of account is sometimes called the "noise plus bias" explanation.[1] According to the better-than-average effect, people have a general tendency to rate their abilities, attributes, and personality traits as better than average.[25] [26] [1] This differs from the Dunning-Kruger effect since it does not track how this overly positive outlook relates to the skill of the people assessing themselves, while the Dunning-Kruger effect mainly focuses on how this type of misjudgment happens for poor performers.[3] [5] [1] When the better-than-average effect is paired with regression toward the mean, it can be explained both that unskilled people tend to greatly overestimate their competence and that the reverse effect for highly skilled people is much less pronounced.[7] [8] By choosing the right variables for the randomness due to luck and a positive offset to account for the better-than-average effect, it is possible to simulate experiments that show almost the same correlation between self-assessed ability and objective performance as found in the empirical research.[7] But even proponents of this explanation agree that this does not explain the empirical findings in full. This means that the Dunning-Kruger effect may still have a role to play, if only a minor one.[7] Opponents of this approach have argued that this explanation can account for the Dunning-Kruger effect only when assessing one's ability relative to one's peer group but not when the self-assessment happens relative to an objective standard.[8] [1]
Practical significance [edit]
Various claims have been made about the Dunning-Kruger effect's practical significance or why it matters. They often focus on how it causes the affected people to make decisions that lead to bad consequences for them or other people. This is especially relevant for decisions that have long-term consequences. For example, it can lead poor performers into careers for which they are unfit.[7] High performers underestimating their skills, on the other hand, may forego viable career opportunities matching their skills in favor of less promising ones that are below their skill level.[7] In other cases, the bad decisions can also have serious short-term effects, as when overconfidence leads a pilot to operate a new aircraft for which they lack adequate training or to engage in flight maneuvers that exceed their proficiency.[5] Emergency medicine is another area where the correct assessment of one's skills and of the risks of a treatment is of central importance. Tendencies of physicians in training to be overconfident have to be taken into consideration to ensure the appropriate degree of supervision and feedback.[10] The Dunning-Kruger effect can also have negative implications for the agent in a variety of economic activities, in which the price of a good, such as a used car, is often lowered by the buyers' uncertainty about its quality.[3] An overconfident agent unaware of their lack of knowledge, on the other hand, may be willing to pay a much higher price without being conscious of all the potential flaws and risks relevant to the price.[3]
Another implication concerns fields in which self-assessments play an important role in evaluating skills. They are commonly used, for example, in vocational counseling or to estimate the information literacy skills of students and professionals.[7] [4] The Dunning-Kruger effect indicates that such self-assessments often do not correspond to the underlying skills, thereby rendering them unreliable as a method for gathering this type of data.[4] Independent of the field of the skill in question, the meta-cognitive ignorance often associated with the Dunning-Kruger effect may inhibit low performers from improving themselves. Since they are unaware of many of their flaws, they may have little motivation to address and overcome them.[1]
But not all accounts of the Dunning-Kruger effect focus on its negative sides. Some also concentrate on its positive sides, e.g., that ignorance can sometimes be bliss. In this sense, optimism can lead people to experience their situation more positively and overconfidence may help them achieve even unrealistic goals.[1] To distinguish the negative from the positive sides, it has been suggested that two important phases are relevant for realizing a goal: preparatory planning and the execution of the plan.[1] Overconfidence may be beneficial in the execution phase by increasing motivation and energy. But it can be detrimental in the planning phase since the agent may ignore bad odds, take unnecessary risks, or fail to prepare for contingencies.[1] For example, being overconfident may be advantageous for a general on the day of battle because of the additional inspiration passed on to his troops but disadvantageous in the weeks before by ignoring the need for reserve troops or protective gear.[1]
Popular recognition [edit]
In 2000, Kruger and Dunning were awarded a satiric Ig Nobel Prize in recognition of the scientific work recorded in "their modest report".[27] "The Dunning–Kruger Song"[28] is part of The Incompetence Opera,[29] a mini-opera that premiered at the Ig Nobel Prize ceremony in 2017.[30] The mini-opera is billed as "a musical encounter with the Peter principle and the Dunning–Kruger Effect".[31]
See also [edit]
- Big-fish–little-pond effect – People feel better about themselves when they are more obviously superior
- Cognitive dissonance – Stress from contradictory beliefs
- Curse of knowledge – Cognitive bias of assuming that others have the same background to understand
- Four stages of competence – Psychological states when gaining a skill
- Grandiose delusions – Subtype of delusion
- Hanlon's razor – Adage to assume stupidity over malice
- Hubris – Extreme pride or overconfidence, often in combination with arrogance
- Illusion of explanatory depth – Form of cognitive bias
- Impostor syndrome – Psychological pattern of doubting one's accomplishments and fearing being exposed as a "fraud"
- Narcissism – Personality trait of self-love of a perceived perfect self
- Narcissistic personality disorder – Pattern of exaggerated feelings of self-importance
- Not even wrong – Based on invalid reasoning or premises that cannot be proved or disproved
- Optimism bias – Type of cognitive bias
- Overconfidence effect – Personal cognitive bias
- Self-deception – Pretense of virtue; failure to follow one's own expressed moral principles
- Self-efficacy – Psychology concept
- Self-serving bias – Distortion to enhance self-esteem, or to see oneself overly favorably
- Superiority complex – Psychological defense mechanism articulated by Alfred Adler
- Susan Stebbing – British philosopher (1885–1943) – whose writing in 1939 described a similar phenomenon to Dunning–Kruger
- True self and false self – Psychological concepts often used in connection with narcissism
- Ultracrepidarianism – Passing judgment beyond one's expertise
- Law of triviality – Focusing on what is irrelevant but easy to understand
- I know that I know nothing – Famous saying by Socrates
- Pygmalion effect – Phenomenon in psychology
- Gartner hype cycle – Graphical presentation of the maturity of specific technologies – applying a similar model to technologies' life cycle
References [edit]
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae af ag ah ai aj ak Dunning, David (1 January 2011). "Chapter Five – The Dunning–Kruger Effect: On Being Ignorant of One's Own Ignorance". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 44. Academic Press. pp. 247–296. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00005-6. ISBN978-0123855220. Archived from the original on 29 May 2020. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n "Dunning-Kruger effect". www.britannica.com. Archived from the original on 30 November 2021. Retrieved 7 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Schlösser, Thomas; Dunning, David; Johnson, Kerri L.; Kruger, Justin (1 December 2013). "How unaware are the unskilled? Empirical tests of the "signal extraction" counter-explanation for the Dunning–Kruger effect in self-evaluation of performance". Journal of Economic Psychology. 39: 85–100. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2013.07.004. ISSN 0167-4870. Archived from the original on 15 May 2022. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
- ^ a b c Mahmood, Khalid (1 January 2016). "Do People Overestimate Their Information Literacy Skills? A Systematic Review of Empirical Evidence on the Dunning-Kruger Effect". Communications in Information Literacy. 10 (2): 199–213. doi:10.7548/cil.v10i2.385 (inactive 28 February 2022). Archived from the original on 27 February 2022. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of February 2022 (link) - ^ a b c d e Pavel, Samuel; Robertson, Michael; Harrison, Bryan (October 2012). "The Dunning-Kruger Effect and SIUC University's Aviation Students". Journal of Aviation Technology and Engineering. 2 (1): 125–129. doi:10.5703/1288284314864.
- ^ a b c Litvak, P.; Lerner, J. S. (2009). "Cognitive Bias". The Oxford Companion to Emotion and the Affective Sciences. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on 2 November 2021. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Gignac, Gilles E.; Zajenkowski, Marcin (1 May 2020). "The Dunning-Kruger effect is (mostly) a statistical artefact: Valid approaches to testing the hypothesis with individual differences data". Intelligence. 80: 101449. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2020.101449. ISSN 0160-2896. S2CID 216410901. Archived from the original on 15 May 2022. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p McIntosh, Robert D.; Fowler, Elizabeth A.; Lyu, Tianjiao; Della Sala, Sergio (November 2019). "Wise up: Clarifying the role of metacognition in the Dunning-Kruger effect" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 148 (11): 1882–1897. doi:10.1037/xge0000579. hdl:20.500.11820/b5c09c5f-d2f2-4f46-b533-9e826ab85585. PMID 30802096. S2CID 73460013. Archived (PDF) from the original on 10 January 2022. Retrieved 23 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g h Mazor, Matan; Fleming, Stephen M. (June 2021). "The Dunning-Kruger effect revisited". Nature Human Behaviour. 5 (6): 677–678. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01101-z. ISSN 2397-3374. PMID 33833426. S2CID 233191867. Archived from the original on 7 December 2021. Retrieved 7 December 2021.
- ^ a b c d e TenEyck, Lisa (2021). "20. Dunning-Kruger Effect". Decision Making in Emergency Medicine: Biases, Errors and Solutions. Springer Nature. ISBN978-981-16-0143-9. Archived from the original on 20 December 2021. Retrieved 20 December 2021.
- ^ a b c Kruger, Justin; Dunning, David (1999). "Unskilled and Unaware of It: How Difficulties in Recognizing One's Own Incompetence Lead to Inflated Self-Assessments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (6): 1121–1134. CiteSeerX10.1.1.64.2655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1121. PMID 10626367.
- ^ Dunning, David; Johnson, Kerri; Ehrlinger, Joyce; Kruger, Justin (1 June 2003). "Why People Fail to Recognize Their Own Incompetence". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (3): 83–87. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01235. S2CID 2720400.
- ^ Lee, Chris (5 November 2016). "Revisiting why incompetents think they're awesome". Ars Technica. p. 3. Archived from the original on 19 December 2019. Retrieved 11 January 2014.
- ^ Dunning, David (2005). Self-insight: Roadblocks and Detours on the Path to Knowing Thyself. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 14–15. ISBN978-1841690742. OCLC 56066405.
- ^ Morris, Errol (20 June 2010). "The Anosognosic's Dilemma: Something's Wrong but You'll Never Know What It Is (Part 1)". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 22 June 2010. Retrieved 7 March 2011.
- ^ David Dunning (2011). "The Dunning–Kruger Effect: On Being Ignorant of One's Own Ignorance". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 44: 247–296. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00005-6.
3.1. Definition. Specifically, for any given skill, some people have more expertise and some have less, some a good deal less. What about those people with low levels of expertise? Do they recognize it? According to the argument presented here, people with substantial deficits in their knowledge or expertise should not be able to recognize those deficits. Despite potentially making error after error, they should tend to think they are doing just fine. In short, those who are incompetent, for lack of a better term, should have little insight into their incompetence—an assertion that has come to be known as the Dunning–Kruger effect (Kruger & Dunning, 1999).
- ^ David Dunning; Erik G. Helzer (2014). "Beyond the Correlation Coefficient in Studies of Self-Assessment Accuracy: Commentary on Zell & Krizan (2014)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (2): 126–130. doi:10.1177/1745691614521244. PMID 26173250. S2CID 23729134.
In other words, the best way to improve self-accuracy is simply to make everybody better performers. Doing so helps them to avoid the type of outcome they seem unable to anticipate. Discerning readers will recognize this as an oblique restatement of the Dunning–Kruger effect (see Dunning, 2011; Kruger & Dunning, 1999), which suggests that poor performers are not in a position to recognize the shortcomings in their performance.
- ^ Ames, Daniel R.; Kammrath, Lara K. (September 2004). "Mind-Reading and Metacognition: Narcissism, not Actual Competence, Predicts Self-Estimated Ability" (PDF). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 28 (3): 187–209. CiteSeerX10.1.1.413.8323. doi:10.1023/b:jonb.0000039649.20015.0e. ISSN 0191-5886. S2CID 13376290. Archived (PDF) from the original on 30 October 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2013.
- ^ Ehrlinger, Joyce; Dunning, David (January 2003). "How chronic self-views influence (and potentially mislead) estimates of performance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (1): 5–17. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.5. PMID 12518967.
- ^ Burson, Katherine A.; Larrick, Richard P.; Klayman, Joshua (2006). "Skilled or unskilled, but still unaware of it: How perceptions of difficulty drive miscalibration in relative comparisons". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (1): 60–77. CiteSeerX10.1.1.178.7774. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.60. hdl:2027.42/39168. PMID 16448310.
- ^
- ^ a b c Krajc, Marian; Ortmann, Andreas (1 November 2008). "Are the unskilled really that unaware? An alternative explanation". Journal of Economic Psychology. 29 (5): 724–738. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.12.006. ISSN 0167-4870. Archived from the original on 23 January 2022. Retrieved 24 December 2021.
- ^
- ^ Ackerman, Phillip L.; Beier, Margaret E.; Bowen, Kristy R. (1 September 2002). "What we really know about our abilities and our knowledge". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (4): 587–605. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00174-X. ISSN 0191-8869. Archived from the original on 26 July 2021. Retrieved 26 July 2021.
- ^ Kim, Young-Hoon; Kwon, Heewon; Chiu, Chi-Yue (2017). "The Better-Than-Average Effect Is Observed Because "Average" Is Often Construed as Below-Median Ability". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 898. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00898. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC5479883. PMID 28690555.
- ^ Alicke, M. D.; Govorun, O. (2005). "The Better-Than-Average Effect.". The Self in Social Judgment. Psychology Press. Archived from the original on 24 December 2021. Retrieved 24 December 2021.
- ^ "Ig Nobel Past Winners". Improbable Research. August 2006. Archived from the original on 9 January 2010. Retrieved 6 September 2021.
- ^ "The Dunning–Kruger Song", from The Incompetence Opera . YouTube.com. ImprobableResearch. 15 January 2018. Retrieved 18 January 2018.
- ^ The Incompetence Opera. YouTube.com. ImprobableResearch. 29 December 2017. Retrieved 18 January 2018.
- ^ "The 27th First Annual Ig Nobel Prize Ceremony & Lectures". Archived from the original on 19 January 2018. Retrieved 18 January 2018.
- ^ "Preview: "The Incompetence Opera"". Improbable Research. 30 August 2017. Archived from the original on 19 January 2018. Retrieved 18 January 2018.
Further reading [edit]
- Dunning, David (27 October 2014). "We Are All Confident Idiots". Pacific Standard. The Social Justice Foundation. Retrieved 28 October 2014.
External links [edit]
Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dunning%E2%80%93Kruger_effect
0 Response to "Funny Vine Look at This Graph"
Post a Comment